Article published in:
Interaction Studies
Vol. 21:3 (2020) ► pp. 353386
References

References

Albert, S., & Kessler, S.
(1976) Processes for ending social encounters: The conceptual archaeology of a temporal place. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 6(2), 147–170. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, K., & Whiten, A.
(2003) Grooming interactions among the chimpanzees of the Budongo Forest, Uganda: tests of five explanatory models. Behaviour, 140(4), 519–552. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Aureli, F., Cords, M., & Van Schaik, C. P.
(2002) Conflict resolution following aggression in gregarious animals: a predictive framework. Animal Behaviour, 64(3), 325–343. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Badrian, A. & Badrian, N.
(1984) Social Organization of Pan paniscus in the Lomako Forest, Zaire. In R. L. Susman (Eds) The Pygmy Chimpanzee: Evolutionary Biology and Behavior (pp. 325–346). New York: Plenum Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bangerter, A., Chevalley, E., & Derouwaux, S.
(2010) Managing third-party interruptions in conversations: Effects of duration and conversational role. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 29(2), 235–244. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bangerter, A., & Clark, H. H.
(2003) Navigating joint projects with dialogue. Cognitive Science, 27(2), 195–225. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bangerter, A., H. Clark, H. H., & Katz, A. R.
(2004) Navigating joint projects in telephone conversations. Discourse Processes, 37(1), 1–23. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, L., Gaynor, D., & Henzi, S. P.
(2002) A dynamic interaction between aggression and grooming reciprocity among female chacma baboons. Animal Behaviour, 63(6), 1047–1053. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bekoff, M., & Allen, C.
(1998) Intentional communication and social play: how and why animals negotiate and agree to play. Animal Play: Evolutionary, Comparative, and Ecological Perspectives, 97–114. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bermejo, M., & Omedes, A.
(1999) Preliminary vocal repertoire and vocal communication of wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) at Lilungu (Democratic Republic of Congo). Folia Primatologica, 70(6), 328–357. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C., & Boesch, H.
(1989) Hunting behavior of wild chimpanzees in the Taï National Park. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 78(4), 547–573. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C.
(2002) Cooperative hunting roles among Tai chimpanzees. Human Nature, 13(1), 27–46. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2005) Joint cooperative hunting among wild chimpanzees: Taking natural observations seriously. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(5), 692–693. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2008) Taking development and ecology seriously when comparing cognition: reply to Tomasello and Call (2008). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 122(4), 453–455. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bohn, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M.
(2016) The role of past interactions in great apes’ communication about absent entities. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 130(4), 351–357. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bratman, M. E.
(1993) Shared intention. Ethics, 104(1), 97–113. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Broth, M., & Mondada, L.
(2013) Walking away: The embodied achievement of activity closings in mobile interaction. Journal of Pragmatics, 47(1), 41–58. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C.
(1987) Politeness: Some universals in language usage. (Vol. 4). Cambridge university press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bullinger, A. F., Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M.
(2011) Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, prefer individual over collaborative strategies towards goals. Animal Behaviour, 82(5), 1135–1141. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Call, J., & Tomasello, M.
(2008) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(5), 187–192. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M., & Call, J.
(2013) How joint is the joint attention of apes and human infants. Agency and Joint Attention, 49–61. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cartmill, E. A., & Byrne, R. W.
(2007) Orangutans modify their gestural signaling according to their audience’s comprehension. Current Biology, 17(15), 1345–1348. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chevalley, E., & Bangerter, A.
(2010) Suspending and reinstating joint activities with dialogue. Discourse Processes, 47(4), 263–291. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A.
(2008) Supersizing the Mind: Embodiment, Action, and Cognitive Extension. Oxford University Press, USA. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H.
(1996) Using language. Cambridge university press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2006) Social actions, social commitments. Roots of Human Sociality: Culture, Cognition and Interaction, 126–150.Google Scholar
Clay, Z. & de Waal, F. B.
(2014) Sex and strife: post-conflict sexual contacts in bonobos. Behaviour; 152 (313–334).Google Scholar
(2013) Bonobos respond to distress in others: consolation across the age spectrum. PloS One 8, e55206; CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clutton-Brock, T.
(2009) Cooperation between non-kin in animal societies. Nature, 462(7269), 51–57. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cordoni, G., & Palagi, E.
(2011) Ontogenetic trajectories of chimpanzee social play: similarities with humans. PLoS One, 6(11), e27344. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., Langergraber, K., Ziegler, T. E., Zuberbühler, K., & Deschner, T.
(2013) Urinary oxytocin and social bonding in related and unrelated wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 280(1755), 20122765–20122765. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., Mundry, R., & Zuberbühler, K.
(2012) Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger. Current Biology, 22(2), 142–146. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
De Ruiter, J. P., & Levinson, S. C.
(2008) A biological infrastructure for communication underlies the cultural evolution of languages. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(5), 518–518. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
De Stefani, E., & Mondada, L.
(2018) Encounters in public space: How acquainted versus unacquainted persons establish social and spatial arrangements. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 51(3), 248–270. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
de Waal, F. B.
(1987) Tension regulation and nonreproductive functions on sex in captive bonobos (Pan paniscus). National Geographic Research, 3, 318–335.Google Scholar
(1988) The communicative repertoire of captive bonobos (Pan paniscus), compared to that of chimpanzees. Behaviour, 183–251. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1989) Food sharing and reciprocal obligations among chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 18(5), 433–459. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1990) Peacemaking among primates. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Demuru, E., Ferrari, P. F., & Palagi, E.
(2015) Emotionality and intentionality in bonobo playful communication. Animal Cognition, 18(1), 333–344. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Drea, C. M., Hawk, J. E., & Glickman, S. E.
(1996) Aggression decreases as play emerges in infant spotted hyaenas: preparation for joining the clan. Animal Behaviour, 51(6), 1323–1336. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. I.
(1991) Functional significance of social grooming in primates. Folia Primatologica, 57(3), 121–131. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2010) The social role of touch in humans and primates: behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(2), 260–268. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Duranti, A.
(1997) Universal and culture-specific properties of greetings. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 7(1), 63–97. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Enomoto, T.
(1990) Social play and sexual behavior of the bonobo (Pan paniscus) with special reference to flexibility. Primates, 31(4), 469–480. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fedurek, P., & Dunbar, R. I.
(2009) What does mutual grooming tell us about why chimpanzees groom? Ethology, 115(6), 566–575. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fedurek, P., Slocombe, K. E., Hartel, J. A., & Zuberbühler, K.
(2015) Chimpanzee lip-smacking facilitates cooperative behaviour. Scientific Reports, 5, 13460. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fletcher, G. E., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M.
(2012) Differences in cognitive processes underlying the collaborative activities of children and chimpanzees. Cognitive Development, 27(2), 136–153. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fröhlich, M., Kuchenbuch, P., Müller, G., Fruth, B., Furuichi, T., Wittig, R. M., & Pika, S.
(2016a) Unpeeling the layers of language: Bonobos and chimpanzees engage in cooperative turn-taking sequences. Scientific Reports, 6, 25887. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fröhlich, M., Müller, G., Zeiträg, C., Wittig, R. M., & Pika, S.
(2017) Gestural development of chimpanzees in the wild: the impact of interactional experience. Animal Behaviour, 134, 271–282. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fröhlich, M., Wittig, R. M., & Pika, S.
(2016b) Play-solicitation gestures in chimpanzees in the wild: flexible adjustment to social circumstances and individual matrices. Open Science, 3(8), 160278.Google Scholar
(2016c) Should I stay or should I go? Initiation of joint travel in mother–infant dyads of two chimpanzee communities in the wild. Animal Cognition, 19(3), 483–500. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Furuichi, T.
(1989) Social interactions and the life history of female Pan paniscus in Wamba, Zaire. International Journal of Primatology, 10, 173–197. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, R. A., & Gardner, B. T.
(1969) Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee. Science, 165(3894), 664–672. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gelblum, A., Pinkoviezky, I., Fonio, E., Ghosh, A., Gov, N. & Feinerman, O.
(2015) Ant group optimally amplify the effect of transiently informed individuals. Nature communications, 6, e7729. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Genty, E., Breuer, T., Hobaiter, C., & Byrne, R. W.
(2009) Gestural communication of the gorilla (Gorilla gorilla): repertoire, intentionality and possible origins. Animal Cognition, 12(3), 527–546. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Genty, E., Clay, Z., Hobaiter, C., & Zuberbühler, K.
(2014) Multi-modal use of a socially directed call in bonobos. PLoS One, 9(1), e84738. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Genty, E., Neumann, C., & Zuberbühler, K.
(2015a) Bonobos modify communication signals according to recipient familiarity. Scientific Reports, 5, 16442. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2015b) Complex patterns of signalling to convey different social goals of sex in bonobos, Pan paniscus . Scientific Reports, 5, 16135. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Genty, E., & Zuberbühler, K.
(2014) Spatial reference in a bonobo gesture. Current Biology, 24(14), 1601–1605. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, M.
(1990) Walking together: A paradigmatic social phenomenon. Midwest Studies In Philosophy, 15(1), 1–14. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goffman, E.
(1963) Behavior in public places (New York: Free Press).Google Scholar
(1967) On face-work. Interaction Ritual: Essays on Face-to-Face Behavior, 5–46.Google Scholar
(1981) Forms of talk. University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Göncü, A., Gaskins, S., & Gaskins, S.
(2007) Play and Development: Evolutionary, Sociocultural, and Functional Perspectives (London: Lawrence Erlbaum). CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goodall, J.
(1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe: patterns of behavior. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goodwin, C., & Goodwin, M. H.
(2004) Participation. A companion to linguistic anthropology, 222–244.Google Scholar
Goodwin, C.
(2007) Participation, stance and affect in the organization of activities. Discourse & Society, 18(1), 53–73. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Graham, K. E., Furuichi, T., & Byrne, R. W.
(2017) The gestural repertoire of the wild bonobo (Pan paniscus): a mutually understood communication system. Animal Cognition, 20(2), 171–177. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Graham, K. E., Hobaiter, C., Ounsley, J., Furuichi, T., & Byrne, R. W.
(2018) Bonobo and chimpanzee gestures overlap extensively in meaning. PLOS Biology, 16(2), e2004825. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Graham, K. L., & Burghardt, G. M.
(2010) Current perspectives on the biological study of play: signs of progress. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 85(4), 393–418. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, J. R., Hamann, K., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M.
(2010) Chimpanzee helping in collaborative and noncollaborative contexts. Animal Behaviour, 80(5), 873–880. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grice, H. P.
(1975) Logic and Conversation. In Syntax and Semantics (Vol. 3). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
H. Clark, H. H., & French, J. W.
(1981) Telephone goodbyes. Language in Society, 10(1), 1–19. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hamann, K., Warneken, F., Greenberg, J. R., & Tomasello, M.
(2011) Collaboration encourages equal sharing in children but not in chimpanzees. Nature, 476(7360), 328–331. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hamann, K., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M.
(2012) Children’s developing commitments to joint goals. Child Development, 83(1), 137–145. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hare, B., Melis, A. P., Woods, V., Hastings, S., & Wrangham, R.
(2007) Tolerance allows bonobos to outperform chimpanzees on a cooperative task. Current Biology, 17(7), 619–623. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hare, B., & Tomasello, M.
(2004) Chimpanzees are more skilful in competitive than in cooperative cognitive tasks. Animal Behaviour, 68(3), 571–581. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hayaki, H.
(1985) Social play of juvenile and adolescent chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates, 26(4), 343–360. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Heesen, R., Genty, E., Rossano, F., Zuberbühler, K., & Bangerter, A.
(2017) Social play as joint action: A framework to study the evolution of shared intentionality as an interactional achievement. Learning & Behavior, 45(4), 390–405. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hirata, S., Morimura, N., & Fuwa, K.
(2010) Intentional communication and comprehension of the partner’s role in experimental cooperative tasks. The Mind of the Chimpanzee: Ecological and Experimental Perspectives, 251.Google Scholar
Hobaiter, C., & Byrne, R. W.
(2011) The gestural repertoire of the wild chimpanzee. Animal Cognition, 14(5), 745–767. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2014) The meanings of chimpanzee gestures. Current Biology, 14(24), 1596–1600. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hobaiter, C., Leavens, D. A., & Byrne, R. W.
(2014) Deictic gesturing in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)? Some possible cases. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 128(1), 82. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hohmann, G. & Fruth, B.
(2000) Use and function of genital contacts among female bonobos. Animal Behaviour, 60, (107–120).Google Scholar
Hohmann, G., Mundry, R. & Deschner, T.
(2009) The relationship between socio-sexual behavior and salivary cortisol in bonobos: tests of the tension regulation hypothesis. American Journal of Primatology, 71, 223–232. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hostetter, A. B., Russell, J. L., Freeman, H., & Hopkins, W. D.
(2007) Now you see me, now you don’t: evidence that chimpanzees understand the role of the eyes in attention. Animal Cognition, 10(1), 55. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jaeggi, A. V., Burkart, J. M., & Van Schaik, C. P.
(2010) On the psychology of cooperation in humans and other primates: combining the natural history and experimental evidence of prosociality. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365(1553), 2723–2735. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kaburu, S. S., & Newton-Fisher, N. E.
(2016) Bystanders, parcelling, and an absence of trust in the grooming interactions of wild male chimpanzees. Scientific Reports, 6, 20634. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kano, T.
(1992) The last ape: Pygmy chimpanzee behavior and ecology. Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Kendon, A.
(1976) The F-formation system: The spatial organization of social encounters. Man-Environment Systems, 6, 291–296.Google Scholar
(2004) Gesture: Visible action as utterance. Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kern, A., & Moll, H.
(2017) On the transformative character of collective intentionality and the uniqueness of the human. Philosophical Psychology, 30(3), 319–337. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
King, B. J.
(2009) The dynamic dance: Nonvocal communication in African great apes. Harvard University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M.
(2016) Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs. Science, 354(6308), 110–114. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kuroda, S.
(1984) Interaction over Food among pygmy chimpanzees. In R. L. Susman (Eds) The Pygmy Chimpanzee (pp. 301–324). Springer, Boston, MA. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Leavens, D. A., Bard, K. A., & Hopkins, W. D.
(2017) The mismeasure of ape social cognition. Animal Cognition. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Leavens, D. A., & Hopkins, W. D.
(1998) Intentional communication by chimpanzees: a cross-sectional study of the use of referential gestures. Developmental Psychology, 34(5), 813. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Leavens, D. A., Hopkins, W. D., & Thomas, R. K.
(2004) Referential communication by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118(1), 48–57. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Leavens, D. A., Hostetter, A. B., Wesley, M. J., & Hopkins, W. D.
(2004) Tactical use of unimodal and bimodal communication by chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Animal Behaviour, 67(3), 467–476. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S. C.
(2006) On the human “interaction engine.” In S. C. Levinson & N. J. Enfield (Eds.), Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition and interaction (pp. 39–69). Oxford: Berg.Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C., & Holler, J.
(2014) The origin of human multi-modal communication. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B, 369. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Liebal, K., Call, J., Tomasello, M., & Pika, S.
(2004) To move or not to move: how apes adjust to the attentional state of others. Interaction Studies, 5(2), 199–219. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Logue, D. M., & Stivers, T.
(2012) Squawk in interaction: a primer of conversation analysis for students of animal communication. Behaviour, 149(13–14), 1283–1298. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lyn, H., Russell, J. L., Leavens, D. A., Bard, K. A., Boysen, S. T., Schaeffer, J. A., & Hopkins, W. D.
(2014) Apes communicate about absent and displaced objects: methodology matters. Animal Cognition, 17(1), 85–94. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Machanda, Z. P., Gilby, I. C., & Wrangham, R. W.
(2014) Mutual grooming among adult male chimpanzees: the immediate investment hypothesis. Animal Behaviour, 87, 165–174. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
MacLean, E., & Hare, B.
(2013) Spontaneous triadic engagement in bonobos (Pan paniscus) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 127(3), 245–255. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Melis, A. P., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M.
(2006a) Chimpanzees recruit the best collaborators. Science, 311(5765), 1297–1300. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2006b) Engineering cooperation in chimpanzees: tolerance constraints on cooperation. Animal Behaviour, 72(2), 275–286. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M.
(2013) Chimpanzees’(Pan troglodytes) strategic helping in a collaborative task. Biology Letters, 9(2), 20130009. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Melis, A. P., Warneken, F., Jensen, K., Schneider, A.-C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M.
(2011) Chimpanzees help conspecifics obtain food and non-food items. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278(1710), 1405–1413. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Miles, H. L.
(1990) The cognitive foundations for reference in a signing orangutan. In S. T. Parker & K. R. Gibson (Eds.), “Language” and intelligence in monkeys and apes: Comparative developmental perspectives (pp. 511–539). New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, J. C., & Watts, D. P.
(2001) Why do chimpanzees hunt and share meat? Animal Behaviour, 61(5), 915–924. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Moll, H., & Tomasello, M.
(2007) Cooperation and human cognition: the Vygotskian intelligence hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 362(1480), 639–648. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mondada, L.
(2009) Emergent focused interactions in public places: A systematic analysis of the multimodal achievement of a common interactional space. Journal of Pragmatics, 41(10), 1977–1997. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2011) Understanding as an embodied, situated and sequential achievement in interaction. Journal of Pragmatics, 43(2), 542–552. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Muller, M. N., & Mitani, J. C.
(2005) Conflict and cooperation in wild chimpanzees. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 35, 275–331. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Neumann, C., & Kulik, L.
(2014) EloRating-a brief tutorial. R Package Version, 43, 1–26.Google Scholar
Palagi, E.
(2008) Sharing the motivation to play: the use of signals in adult bonobos. Animal Behaviour, 75(3), 887–896. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2006) Social play in bonobos (Pan paniscus) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Implications for natural social systems and interindividual relationships. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 129(3), 418–426. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Palagi, E., Cordoni, G., & Borgognini Tarli, S. M.
(2004) Immediate and delayed benefits of play behaviour: new evidence from chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Ethology, 110(12), 949–962. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Parish, A. R.
(1994) Sex and food control in the ‘uncommon chimpanzee’: how bonobo females overcome a phylogenetic legacy of male dominance. Ethology. and Sociobiology, 15 (157–179).Google Scholar
Pellis, S. M., & Pellis, V. C.
(1996) On knowing it’s only play: the role of play signals in play fighting. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 1(3), 249–268. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S.
(2004) Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(2), 169–190. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pika, S., & Mitani, J.
(2006) Referential gestural communication in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Current Biology, 16(6), R191–R192. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pika, S., & Zuberbühler, K.
(2008) Social games between bonobos and humans: evidence for shared intentionality? American Journal of Primatology, 70(3), 207–210. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Poirier, F. E., & Smith, E. O.
(1974) Socializing functions of primate play. American Zoologist, 14(1), 275–287. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Port, M., Clough, D., & Kappeler, P. M.
(2009) Market effects offset the reciprocation of grooming in free-ranging redfronted lemurs, Eulemur fulvus rufus. Animal Behaviour, 77(1), 29–36. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., Nelson, K. E., & Boysen, S. T.
(1992) Comprehension of role reversal in chimpanzees: evidence of empathy? Animal Behaviour, 43(4), 633–640. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, A. I., Vick, S.-J., Roberts, S. G. B., & Menzel, C. R.
(2014) Chimpanzees modify intentional gestures to coordinate a search for hidden food. Nature Communications, 5, 3088. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rosas, A., & Bermúdez, J. P.
(2018) Viewing others as equals: The non-cognitive roots of shared intentionality. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 9(3), 485–502. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rossano, F.
(2013) Sequence organization and timing of bonobo mother-infant interactions. Interaction Studies, 14(2), 160–189. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2013) Gaze in Conversation. The handbook of conversation analysis, 308–329.Google Scholar
Rossano, F., & Liebal, K.
(2014) Requests’ and ‘offers’ in orangutans and human infants. Requesting in social interaction, 333–362.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, S., McDonald, K., Sevcik, R. A., Hopkins, W. D., & Rubert, E.
(1986) Spontaneous symbol acquisition and communicative use by pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115(3), 211. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. R.
(1990) Collective intentions and actions. In P. Cohen, J. Morgan, & M. Pollack (Eds.), Intentions in communication (pp. 401–415). Cambridge, MA:MIT Press.Google Scholar
Schegloff, E. A.
(1986) The routine as achievement. Human Studies, 9(2–3), 111–151. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schegloff, E. A., & Sacks, H.
(1973) Opening up closings. Semiotica, 8(4), 289–327. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schino, G., & Aureli, F.
(2009) Reciprocal altruism in primates: partner choice, cognition, and emotions. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 39, 45–69. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schino, G., di Sorrentino, E. P., & Tiddi, B.
(2007) Grooming and coalitions in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata): partner choice and the time frame reciprocation. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 121(2), 181. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sebanz, M. J., & Knoblich, G.
(2016) The Sense of Commitment: A Minimal Approach. Frontiers in psychology, 6 1968 doi:  CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L.
(1984) Grooming, alliances and reciprocal altruism in vervet monkeys. Nature, 308(5959), 541. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Slocombe, K. E., & Zuberbühler, K.
(2005) Functionally referential communication in a chimpanzee. Current Biology, 15(19), 1779–1784. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Surbeck, M., & Hohmann, G.
(2015) Social preferences influence the short-term exchange of social grooming among male bonobos. Animal Cognition, 18(2), 573–579. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tan, J., Ariely, D., & Hare, B.
(2017) Bonobos respond prosocially toward members of other groups. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 14733. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tan, J., & Hare, B.
(2017) Prosociality among non-kin in bonobos and chimpanzees compared. Bonobos: Unique in Mind, Bain, and Behavior (Eds Hare, B. & Yamamoto, S.), 140–154.Google Scholar
Tartabini, A., & Dienske, H.
(1979) Social play and rank order in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Behavioural Processes, 4(4), 375–383. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tollefsen, D., & Dale, R.
(2012) Naturalizing joint action: A process-based approach. Philosophical Psychology, 25(3), 385–407. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(2008) Origins of human communication. MIT press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009) Why we cooperate. MIT press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2014) A natural history of human thinking. Harvard University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M.
(2005) The emergence of social cognition in three young chimpanzees: III. Understanding intentional action. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.Google Scholar
(2007) Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10(1), 121–125. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H.
(2005) Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(5), 675. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Moll, H.
(2010) The gap is social: Human shared intentionality and culture. Springer.Google Scholar
Townsend, S. W., Koski, S. E., Byrne, R. W., Slocombe, K. E., Bickel, B., Boeckle, M., … Manser, M. B.
(2017) Exorcising Grice’s ghost: an empirical approach to studying intentional communication in animals. Biological Reviews, 92(3), 1427–1433. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Trivers, R. L.
(1971) The evolution of reciprocal altruism. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 46(1), 35–57. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tuomela, R.
(2005) We-intentions revisited. Philosophical Studies, 125(3), 327–369. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Völter, C. J., Rossano, F., & Call, J.
(2015) From exploitation to cooperation: social tool use in orang-utan mother–offspring dyads. Animal Behaviour, 100, 126–134. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Warneken, F., Chen, F., & Tomasello, M.
(2006) Cooperative activities in young children and chimpanzees. Child Development, 77(3), 640–663. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Warneken, F., Hare, B., Melis, A. P., Hanus, D., & Tomasello, M.
(2007) Spontaneous altruism by chimpanzees and young children. PLoS Biology, 5(7), e184. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Watts, D. P., & Mitani, J. C.
(2001) Boundary patrols and intergroup encounters in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour, 138(3), 299–327. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Yamamoto, S., Humle, T., & Tanaka, M.
(2009) Chimpanzees help each other upon request. PLoS One, 4(10), e7416. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Youssouf, I. A., Grimshaw, A. D., & Bird, C. S.
(1976) greetings in the desert. American Ethnologist, 3(4), 797–824. CrossrefGoogle Scholar